What’s the best
way to influence a teenager?
If you’re raising a teenager, you already know the
challenge. And the dangers: careless driving, alcohol and drug abuse,
unprotected sex. Helping them make good decisions – that is, helping them
accurately assess the short- and long-term risks – seems a distant dream. But a
recent study hints at a fresh approach that may influence their behavior.
And the message is simple: focus on positive, not
negative, outcomes. For example, if your teenager has taken up smoking, it’ll
be more helpful to emphasize the benefits of stopping (“you’ll have more money,
and better skin”) than the potential long-term negative consequences (“you’ll
get lung cancer”). Similarly, when
trying to influence teens to cut back on alcohol and drug use, it may be more
effective to emphasize improved sports performance than the long-term health
risks.
In a press release, the authors explained: “. . . People
have a natural tendency to ignore negative information when making decisions, a
trait that may be particularly pertinent to young people, who tend to engage in
more risky and dangerous behavior.” The study, conducted by researchers from
University College London in the UK, was published in the Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences and was funded the Wellcome Trust and the
Royal Society.
The study’s findings, said study author Dr. Christina
Moutsiana, “could help to explain the limited impact of campaigns targeted at
young people to highlight the dangers of careless driving, unprotected sex,
alcohol and drug abuse, and other risky behaviors."
Added co-author Dr. Tali Sharot: "Our findings show
that if you want to get young people to better learn about the risks associated
with their choices, you might want to focus on the benefits that a positive
change would bring rather than hounding them with horror stories."
In the study’s introductory remarks, the authors provided
this broad overview: “Human decision making is markedly influenced by beliefs
of what might occur in the future. We form and update those beliefs based on
information we receive from the world around us. However, even when we are
presented with accurate information, cognitive biases and heuristics restrict
our ability to make adequate adjustments to our prior beliefs.”
How was the study conducted? Participants, ages 9 to 26,
were asked to assess the relative dangers of potential adverse life events
(e.g., car accident, getting lung disease). The researchers then showed
participants the actual statistics for these events and noted how each person
adjusted their belief, after learning that the risk was higher or lower than
they had estimated. The bottom line:
when it’s good news, our beliefs change; when it’s bad, news, not so much.
Said the study authors: “The results show
that younger participants were less likely to learn from information that shows
them that the future is bleaker than expected. In other words, even when they
know the risks, they have difficulties using that information if it's worse
than they thought it would be. By contrast, the ability to learn from good news
remained stable across all ages.”
While buoyed by the findings, Dr. Moutsiana offered this
cautionary note. She told Medical News
Today that “while positive messages about not smoking might be more effective
than negative messages, other factors, such as social pressure, need to be
considered in why teenagers smoke.”
Added Moutsiana, in the Medical News Today article: “"We used
events related more to physical danger. . . . It is possible that events that
relate more to social pressure might have a different effect. Therefore it
needs to be examined in control experiments."
No comments:
Post a Comment